If a tree falls in the forest and no one is
around to hear it, does it make a sound? As is elucidated by the riddle, there are two related but distinct phenomena encompassed by the
term sound. Sound could refer to the qualitative experience of hearing a sound
of which the explanation would be neurological with appeals to electrochemical
mechanisms. In this
case no sound was made by the tree falling. Meanwhile
an explanation of the vibration of air molecules which is then decoded by
various parts of our ear/brain to create the qualitative experience would
appeal to the laws of physics and math. Maybe they’d both appeal to physics one day if you believe
reductionism, but
regardless, in this case the tree most certainly made a sound. In this presentation I seek to connect these two realms in order
to explain how laws of sound are reflected in mechanisms of brain and later in
various musical systems.
1)
Amplitude = volume/loudness
The amount that air
molecules that are displaced
Measured in decibels
2)
Frequency = pitch
Measured in hertz (hz), or periodicity…
Or compressions/expansions per second
Imagine that in the lower waveform the oscillation is
occurring twice for every time it occurs once in the upper waveform. This would
means that if the first waveform had a frequency of 100 hz, then the second one
would be 200 hz, a 2:1 ration, or in music theory terms, an octave.
To hear what this sounds like go to http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Octave
(it plays first two sounds together, then the lower one, then the higher one)
3 Complexity = timbre
Created
by naturally occurring overtones.
Very few things
in the world create pretty waveforms like the ones above. A tuning fork is the
classic example; the sound you get when you press a button on a telephone is a
more modern one. For just about everything else we get multiple waves appear simultaneously, the fundamental frequency and its integer multiples each occurring
with a lower amplitude than the fundamental. The diagram bellow illustrates how this happens
using a string.
1:1 ratio – note
= A
• 880 Hz (440*2)
2:1 ration – note
= A
• 1320 Hz (440*3)
3:2 ratio – note
= E
• 1760 Hz (440*4)
4:3 ratio – note
= D
• 2200 Hz (440*5)
5:4 ratio – note
= C#
Interestingly, our brain
doesn’t consciously perceive all these tones, although you can train yourself to
hear them. The illusion of the missing fundamental demonstrates this property;
you can remove the fundamental frequency but you still hear the vibrating air
molecules as the pitch corresponding to the fundamental frequency. To try it
yourself click the bellow. http://sites.sinauer.com/wolfe3e/chap10/missingfundF.htm
Something that
may not be obvious from the above is what the 2:1 ratio refers to the real
world. Take a guitar and pluck the string, then take a ruler and measure the
string (or just imagine you’re doing it). Cut the distance in half and you’ll
find your finger on the twelfth fret, the only one with two dots on it. Play it
and it will be twice the hz as compared to the same string played without your
finger on it. You might notice this interval (distance between two notes) sound
pleasant or consonant. It’s also the same interval you listened to if you
followed the Wikipedia link above.
Those in
psycho-acoustics argue that it sounds pleasant due to what Helmholtz termed the
coincidence of partials, which translates in laymen’s terms to overlapping
overtones (indeed consonance literally means overlapping sounds). 100 hz and 200
hz tones (a 2:1 ratio) will have overlapping overtones at 400 hz 600 hz and 800
hz. As you can see this
includes the majority of the overtones propagated when these notes are struck. For this
reason notes that are related to one another by a 2:1 ratio not only sound consonant but in fact sound like the
same note just in a
different octave. Most people would agree that something
about these two notes is fundamentally the same despite different pitches and
qualia. This is then reflected in music systems around the world. If men and
woman start singing together, they will naturally sing an octave apart.
Meanwhile, the
other notes with overtones overlapping with the overtones of the 100 hz fundamental frequency will
also sound consonant but not as starkly similar. For example, a 150 hz fundamental
will overlap with 100 hz at 300 hz, 600hz etc, but not 450 hz. The ratio of
150:100 is the same as that of 3:2, or a perfect fifth in music theory terms.
The octave and fifth are present in the majority of music systems around the
world. A pattern emerges already, the smaller ratio of frequencies, the more
overlap in overtones.

•
So the
big question: Why, when creating
scales and chords, melodies and harmonies, do humans employ a relatively small
number of the infinite possibilities?
•
Well
we already determined that what we hear is biased by the overtone series.
Studies have shown that these intervals are then represented in language prosody (the tones we use
in speech to display different emotions) and musical composition (“A Biological
Rationale for Musical Scales”, Gill
and Purves“Musical intervals in speech”,
Ross and Choi). This leads to
accurate decoding of emotions from prosody and music across languages and
cultures (“Universal Recognition of Three Basic Emotions in Music”,
Fritz, Thomas, Jentschke, Gosselin, Sammler, Peretz, Turner, Friederici, and
Koelsch “Recognition of emotion in Japanese, Western, and Hindustani music by
Japanese listeners”, Balkwill, Thompson, and Matsunaga, “Communication of
Emotions in vocal expression and music performance: different channels same
code?”, Juslin, and Laukka).
•
It
doesn’t matter what tuning system you use, whether you have 12 notes, 19 notes,
or 24 notes dividing the octave. Those are cultural variations, overtones are
naturally occurring. Even in those alternate tuning systems, scales are generally
based on 5 or 7 tones that correspond in some predictable way with the overtone
series e.g. demarcate the octave and fifth
as the most stable notes in the scale
•
This
isn’t to say dissonant intervals are not used. Rather, when they are used is
dependent upon norms specified by cultural practices. Furthermore,
cross-cultural variation comes to effect how we perceive and respond to
different musical stimuli in important and documentable ways. One that comes to
mind is the studies of interpolated notes. Imagine we take someone who has been
raised in a western musical system and someone raised in an eastern music tradition. Let’s say the most common eastern scale shares 5/7 notes with the most common western scale.
Play each of these people a melody with just the 5 common
notes, and then afterwords play them a note which was not included in the
melody. If that note occurs in the western scale that encompasses the other 5
notes that were played, the
westerner will say they heard it in the passage, but easterner will not, and vica-versa. We interpolate notes based
on the musical system which we have grown up in. This in turn effects
expectations, the heart of musical understand according to most theorists, and even
modulates what you are actually hearing due to top down processing.
Pitch
|
Frequency (Hz)
|
Half-steps from C
|
Interval with C
|
Ratio with C
|
C
|
262
|
0
|
Unison
|
1:1
|
C sharp/D flat
|
262(2^1/12) = 278
|
1
|
Minor 2nd
|
16:15
|
D
|
262(2^2/12) = 294
|
2
|
Major 2nd
|
9:8
|
D sharp/E flat
|
262(2^3/12) = 312
|
3
|
Minor 3rd
|
6:5
|
E
|
262(2^4/12) = 330
|
4
|
Major 3rd
|
5:4
|
F
|
262(2^5/12) = 350
|
5
|
Perfect 4th
|
4:3
|
F sharp/G flat
|
262(2^6/12) = 371
|
6
|
Tritone
|
45:32
|
G
|
262(2^7/12) = 393
|
7
|
Perfect 5th
|
3:2
|
G sharp/A flat
|
262(2^8/12) = 416
|
8
|
Minor 6th
|
8:5
|
A
|
262(2^9/12) = 440
|
9
|
Major 6th
|
5:3
|
A sharp/ B flat
|
262(2^10/12) = 467
|
10
|
Minor 7th
|
16:9
|
B
|
262(2^11/12) = 495
|
11
|
Major 7th
|
15:8
|
C
|
262(2) = 524
|
12
|
octave
|
2:1
|
No comments:
Post a Comment